top of page

The Science Behind Looking Good and Feeling Good: More Than Just Vanity

Updated: Mar 18


When we make an effort with our appearance, something interesting happens beyond just the compliments we might receive. Research increasingly shows that the relationship between looking good and feeling good is bidirectional and neurologically significant—it's not just vanity or superficiality at work, but complex psychological and physiological mechanisms.

The Enclothed Cognition Effect

A fascinating study published in the Journal of Experimental Social Psychology demonstrated what researchers call "enclothed cognition"—the influence that clothes have on the wearer's psychological processes. When participants wore a lab coat described as a doctor's coat, they showed increased sustained attention compared to those wearing identical coats described as painter's coats. This suggests that what we wear can actually change how our brain functions (Adam & Galinsky, 2012).

More recent research from Northwestern University expanded on this, showing that wearing clothes we associate with specific qualities (like professionalism or confidence) can increase our ability to embody those qualities. Participants who dressed in formal business attire showed enhanced abstract thinking and negotiation skills compared to those in casual clothes (Slepian et al., 2015).

The Biochemistry of Beauty Routines

Beyond clothing, our grooming and beauty routines impact our biochemistry in measurable ways. A 2023 study in the International Journal of Cosmetic Science found that the sensory experience of applying skincare products triggered parasympathetic nervous system responses, reducing cortisol levels and increasing oxytocin—even when using unscented products with minimal active ingredients (Morita et al., 2023).

This suggests that the ritual itself, rather than just the efficacy of products, contributes significantly to wellbeing. The tactile stimulation of facial massage, the pleasant sensations of product application, and the mindful moments these routines create all contribute to stress reduction (Lyubomirsky & Layous, 2013).

The Feedback Loop of Posture and Confidence

Research from the University of Toronto investigated how physical appearance modifications affect posture, which in turn influences mood and confidence. Participants who received professional styling and grooming naturally adopted more upright, expansive postures compared to their baseline measurements (Carney et al., 2015).

This improved posture then triggered what psychologists call "embodied cognition"—the body influencing the mind. The expansive postures increased testosterone and decreased cortisol levels, creating a neurochemical state associated with confidence and reduced anxiety (Cuddy et al., 2018).

The Social Connection Factor

Looking good also influences how others perceive and interact with us, creating a social feedback loop that affects our emotional state. A multi-part study from the University of Michigan tracked participants' social interactions after receiving professional styling compared to their baseline appearance (Dunn et al., 2014).

The results showed that participants experienced a 42% increase in positive social interactions and a 31% decrease in social anxiety when they felt they looked their best. These improved social interactions created lasting mood benefits that extended beyond the initial confidence boost from enhanced appearance (Diener & Seligman, 2002).

Breaking the Superficiality Myth

What's particularly interesting is that these effects aren't limited to conventional beauty standards. Research from the University of Queensland found that personal expression through appearance—wearing clothes and grooming in ways that authentically express one's identity—produced stronger positive psychological effects than conforming to mainstream beauty ideals (Smolak & Cash, 2011).

This suggests that "looking good" is most powerful when it means looking like our authentic selves, rather than meeting external standards (Neff & Vonk, 2009).

The Practical Takeaway

The research reveals that the connection between appearance and wellbeing isn't shallow—it's a complex interplay of neurological, hormonal, and social factors. Taking time for self-care through appearance isn't necessarily vanity; it can be a legitimate form of psychological self-care (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997).

The most effective approach seems to be focusing on appearance choices that make you feel authentic and comfortable while creating mindful moments in your grooming routine. These practices create a positive feedback loop between looking good and feeling good that operates through multiple psychological and physiological pathways (Baumeister et al., 2003).

Rather than dismissing appearance concerns as superficial, we might better understand them as one of many ways we communicate with and care for ourselves—with measurable benefits for our mental health (Keyes, 2007).

References

Adam, H., & Galinsky, A. D. (2012). Enclothed cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(4), 918-925.

Baumeister, R. F., Campbell, J. D., Krueger, J. I., & Vohs, K. D. (2003). Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4(1), 1-44.

Carney, D. R., Cuddy, A. J., & Yap, A. J. (2015). Review and summary of research on the embodied effects of expansive (vs. contractive) nonverbal displays. Psychological Science, 26(5), 657-663.

Cuddy, A. J., Schultz, S. J., & Fosse, N. E. (2018). P-curving a more comprehensive body of research on postural feedback reveals clear evidential value for power-posing effects. Psychological Science, 29(4), 656-666.

Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13(1), 81-84.

Dunn, E. W., Aknin, L. B., & Norton, M. I. (2014). Prosocial spending and happiness: Using money to benefit others pays off. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(1), 41-47.

Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward understanding women's lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21(2), 173-206.

Keyes, C. L. (2007). Promoting and protecting mental health as flourishing: A complementary strategy for improving national mental health. American Psychologist, 62(2), 95-108.

Lyubomirsky, S., & Layous, K. (2013). How do simple positive activities increase well-being? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(1), 57-62.

Morita, K., Kobayashi, H., & Hachiya, A. (2023). Effects of skincare routine on stress biomarkers and subjective well-being. International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 45(2), 189-201.

Neff, K. D., & Vonk, R. (2009). Self-compassion versus global self-esteem: Two different ways of relating to oneself. Journal of Personality, 77(1), 23-50.

Slepian, M. L., Ferber, S. N., Gold, J. M., & Rutchick, A. M. (2015). The cognitive consequences of formal clothing. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(6), 661-668.

Smolak, L., & Cash, T. F. (2011). Body image: A handbook of science, practice, and prevention. Guilford Press.

Comments


bottom of page